- Experiments in Insight Meditation
- Rod Bucknell
This is a report on my experiences with insight meditation (vipassana-bhavana)
during a four-and-a-half year period spent in Thailand and India. It deals only in small
part with my formal vipassana training; that lasted barely two months and is only briefly
summarized. For the most part it describes independent experiments with meditative
techniques which I assumed, at the time, to be original and unorthodox. Subsequent reading
has indicated that those techniques, though not widely practised, are in fact well known
within certain schools of meditation. This has reinforced my belief in the value of the
techniques, and has encouraged me to publish a report on my experience.
Formal Vipassana Training
The vipassana centre in Bangkok where I began my meditative training claimed to
teach the system of practice developed by Mahasi Sayadaw of Myanma, often called Burmese satipatthana.
The practice was based on two related exercises: (1) concentration on a single object,
namely the sensations accompanying the rising and falling movements of the abdomen in
breathing, practised while sitting with eye closed; and (2) constant attention to the
various sensations experienced while engaged in simple activities (walking, eating, etc.),
all of which had to be performed in slow motion. In both of these exercises any
digressions from the assigned object of attention had to be mentally noted. Distracting
sensations were identified as "itching," "pain," "hearing,"
etc., and distracting thoughts as "daydreaming," "planning,"
"theorizing," etc. The two exercises were alternated: half an hour of sitting,
half an hour of walking, and so on throughout most of the eighteen-hour waking day.
Judged by my teacher's comments at our daily interviews, and by the published reports
of other meditators which I subsequently read, my experiences under this rigorous regime
were in most respects typical. Like most beginners in meditation, I initially had great
difficulty in stemming the flow of thought and keeping attention on the prescribed object;
I experienced astonishment and distress on realizing the triviality and worthlessness of
most of my mental content; and I gained occasional useful insights into formerly hidden
layers of my personality.
After two weeks of practice I was able to maintain concentration on the object for a
minute or more at a time. This partial success in concentrating brought with it certain
pleasant experiences. I increasingly found, on opening my eyes and rising from my seat,
that my perception of the world and of myself had undergone subtle changes. Colours,
textures, and shapes seemed to have become unusually clear and vivid; there was a
refreshing newness, interest, and beauty in objects that had formerly been dull and
humdrum; time seemed to have stood still, so that I lived in an eternal present moment --
while the effect lasted; and I felt as if I had been somehow purified of negative emotions
and was radiating benevolence toward all beings. These positive effects gave me
much-needed encouragement, and I redoubled my meditative efforts.
At the end of three weeks I was able to maintain uninterrupted mental one-pointedness
for prolonged periods. During such periods nothing was present in consciousness but the
meditation object, the sensations in the abdomen. The rest of the body, and the world
outside it, had ceased to exist. I identified completely with the sensations: I was
the sensations. Increasingly I experienced synaesthetic effects. For example, I often
"saw" the pattern of sensations in the abdomen in various forms -- usually as an
oscillating system of levers, or as a pulsating globe of light. On my teacher's advice I
took this mental image as my new object of concentration. (The sitting practice had, by
this stage, become the principal component of the meditative regime; mindful walking was
now of secondary importance.) Then one day, as I was concentrating on my pulsating image,
it suddenly disappeared, plunging me into a pitch-back emptiness. My teacher regarded this
strange experience as an important meditative attainment, and told me to cultivate and
prolong it. I followed his instruction for a time -- until I learned that the objective
was to prolong the state of emptiness to twenty-four hours. The achievement of that feat
would constitute successful completion of the meditation course.
At that point I decided it was time to leave the vipassana centre. I had begun to doubt
the value of this state of mental emptiness, and of some of my other hard-won meditative
skills as well. Thanking my teacher, I left Bangkok and moved to Chiangmai in the north of
the country.
In Chiangmai I entered another vipassana centre, to find out if their methods were
significantly different. There were differences in detail, but they amounted simply to
different ways of inducing the same concentrated state. After five days I left. I moved
into a quiet wat (monastery) and, disregarding my former teacher's last words to
me, gave up meditating. There followed a period of reflection and evaluation of the
experience I had been through.
In retrospect my training in vipassana meditation seemed to me the most important and
valuable thing I had ever done. At the same time I found much to criticize. I came to the
conclusion that "insight meditation" was hardly an appropriate term for the kind
of practice I had been engaged in. While there had been some incipient insights, the
nature of the practice had been such as to prevent my following them up. Repeatedly I had
had to abandon promising lines of introspective observation in order to return to the
concentration practice; concentration had always received the primary emphasis. I had once
mentioned my frustration on this point to my teacher. He had told me that such curiosity
about the mechanism of thought must be recognized as an alluring side-track, something
that one had to forgo in order to progress; and he had gently reprimanded me for coming to
the course with preconceptions about how it ought to proceed, and for having less than
total faith in the method. At that time I had been prepared to suppress my introspective
curiosity, to provisionally accept the method on faith, and to press on with concentrating
on my abdomen. Now in retrospect it seemed that the emphasis had been wrongly placed. I
had been taught how to have experiences rather than how to observe or understand
them. For example, by mastering concentration I had experienced a remarkable clarity of
perception and other effects, but I had not gained any insight into the nature or cause of
those experiences. I had not been able to discover what change in the mind's mode of
functioning was responsible for the supernormal clarity of perception, the jamais vu
feeling, and so on. As for the normal, everyday processes of the mind, these remained
almost as much a mystery as ever. I had hoped (here, admittedly, another preconception
taken with me into the course) that I would be taught how to observe mental process from
the inside. But in that direction we had gone no further than labelling digression as
"daydreaming," "planning," etc.
I was unable to be more precise than this in identifying the reasons for my
dissatisfaction with the practice. What I was sure of, however, was that I placed high
value on insight into the mind, and that the course of practice had brought little of
that. I discussed my problem with several monks at the wat, some of them meditators with
years of experience. A few agreed that the emphasis on mental emptiness was a serious
fault, and maintained that it represented a distortion of Mahasi Sayadaw's teaching. They
said that the real objective was insight into the "three universal
characteristics": transience, suffering, and non-selfhood. My teacher had in fact
drawn my attention to these "characteristics"; for example, pains in the
abdomen, formerly unnoticed but revealed during concentration, were evidence of the
universality of suffering. I had been inclined to take such observations rather as
evidence that an individual's interpretation of an altered state of consciousness reflect
his or her religious conditioning. (The three characteristics are repeatedly mentioned in
the Buddhist canon and in monks' teachings.)
One of the monks I consulted condemned the entire Burmese satipatthana method as
lacking textual authority. He advised me to practise instead mindfulness of breathing (anapana-sati),
textually the best authenticated of all meditative techniques. Although this was again a
form of concentration practice, and although I now had neither the guidance of a teacher
nor the conductive environment of a meditation centre, I decided, without much enthusiasm,
to try it.
In mindfulness of breathing as usually practised, the object of concentration is the
fine tactile sensation experienced at the nostril as the breath moves in and out. I found
this subtle object far more difficult to concentrate on than the abdomen had been. This
difficulty, combined with the lack of guidance and my skepticism about the value of
concentration, meant that, in spite of the experience gained in Bangkok, I made slow
progress. But this unpromising situation brought unforeseen benefits; for in the course of
attempting to control my unruly mind, I developed, almost by accident it seemed, a new
meditative technique. This technique led to others, yielding in time my own version of a
course in insight meditation.
Retracing
Like most meditators I had been stuck by the fact that the topics to which my
mind wandered were often totally unrelated to my actual situation, suggesting a lack of
any coherence in the thinking process. I could be concentrating on my breathing one
moment, and the next moment find myself speculating on the mechanical condition of my car,
wondering if my washing was dry yet, or dwelling on a vivid fantasy of biting into a tasty
cheese sandwich. In order to find out something about the unseen process whereby such vast
transitions came about, I introduced a variation into my practice. Instead of returning to
the concentration object directly, as I had been taught to do, I carefully retraced my
mental steps, making the mind return by the way it had come. The result was a
reconstruction, in reverse, of the mental digression.
There is nothing original in this technique. Probably most people have at some time
tired retracing their mental tracks. It is an interesting thing to do if, for example, one
suddenly realizes, in the middle of some task, that one is thinking of something totally
different, and is curious to find out how this came about. The following example (chosen
at random from many such in a recent meditation session ) illustrates how the procedure is
applied in practice.
While attempting to keep my attention focused on the tactile sensation at the rim of my
right nostril, I suddenly realize that I am, instead, pondering on a long-standing, though
essentially trivial problem, namely the lack of sufficient shelves in my office at work.
Instead of returning immediately to the nostril, I reconstruct, in reverse order, the
sequence of thoughts. From my cluttered office, with its inadequate shelves, I go back to
a set of white-painted shelves standing unused in the garage at home; then I go back to a
similar white-painted set of shelves in a friend's house; then to the last occasion on
which I visited that friend, when, seated near the set of shelves, he demonstrated his
limited talent on the cello (the cello bow at one stage actually knocked against the
shelves); then to a concert I once attended, featuring an impressive performance by a very
stout male cellist with piano accompaniment; then to the pianist, regarding whom my
principal impression was that he seemed to slouch instead of sitting upright as a pianist
should; then, finally, to my own posture which in the prolonged meditation session has
become very slouched, producing a slight pain in the back. With the identification of this
pain in my back as the beginning point of the digression, the reconstruction is complete:
aching back -to-: slouching pianist -to-: stout cellist -to-:
friend playing cello near shelves -to-: shelves in garage -to-: office
with inadequate shelves.
Recognizing that this retracing procedure had potential as a means for revealing the
formerly hidden processes of thought, I began practising it regularly in association with
the mindfulness of breathing. My practice then comprised a repeated cycle of three stages:
1. concentration on the chosen object (the breathing);
2. a short thought sequence, as the mind wanders from the object;
3. retracing this thought sequence to its starting point.
Previously the practice had been limited to stages 1 and 2; now, with stage 3 added, it
became possible to discover the nature of the digressions.
Practising in this way, I found that the same course of events was repeated again and
again. Some initial stimulus (in the cited example, the pain of the back) would set in
train a sequence of thoughts, which would rapidly lead far away from the starting point.
By observing the general direction taken by thought sequences, I learned things about
myself that I would otherwise perhaps never have suspected: unresolved conflicts,
previously unrecognized interests, fears, etc. Reviewing this material had a perceptible
therapeutic effect. Problems became less important, and I could smile at aspects of myself
that I had previously taken too seriously. Retracing therefore became the principal
component of my practice. Concentration was now of secondary importance. However, I found
that concentration could not be abandoned altogether; indeed it proved to be an
indispensable part of the meditative procedure, serving as an anchor to prevent the mind
from drifting too far.
Since I was using mindfulness of breathing as the basis for my three-stage practice, I
meditated initially in the accepted cross-legged posture and with eyes shut. Walking (as
in the vipassana course) had no place in my regime. However, I soon decided to introduce
mindful walking, in order to overcome a problem I was having with physical fatigue. I
would walk slowly along a quiet path -- not, however, in the artificial, slow-motion
fashion required by my vipassana teacher -- with eyes directed at the ground and attention
focused on the changing visual pattern before me. Then, each time I realized the mind had
wandered into a train of thought, I would retrace.
This modification of the procedure, prompted initially by considerations of comfort and
practical convenience, proved to have a profound effect on the nature of the meditation.
Because the eyes were now open, the thoughts were seen contrasted with whatever was in the
field of vision. Against that relatively substantial background each thought appeared as a
semi-transparent picture, like a photographic slide projected on to a wall in a well lit
room. I was now seeing my thoughts as mental images. Previously, when I had been
meditating with the eyes closed, each image had occupied the whole of consciousness,
because it had not been contrasted with direct visual sensation in this way. Like a slide
projected on to a screen in a completely darkened room, its seemingly real contents had
completely captured my interest, and consequently its true nature, as an image,
had been overlooked. Now the situation was different. The new effect was exactly as if
there were a slide-projector located somewhere just over my shoulder projecting pictures
on to the path before me. I watched fascinated as each retracing revealed my thoughts as a
"slide-show."
I now modified the mindfulness of breathing practice, meditating thenceforth with my
eyes open and, if at night, always with some form of lighting. The effect was even more
striking than it had been in the walking practice. I saw that the slide-show analogy was
very apt: each image was a faint but nevertheless very realistic reproduction of an
earlier visual experience. Consider, for example, the experience of recalling the stout
cellist. At a certain time in the past I had had the visual experience of seeing the
cellist. Now there was a fainter representation of that visual experience: a picture of
the cellist appeared, as if projected on to the wall before me. The images that appeared
in any particular thought sequence were a tiny selection from the vast number available.
It was as if (pursuing the slide-show analogy) I had an enormous album of slides depicting
my past experiences, from which a few appropriate ones were selected for viewing on any
particular occasion.
Curious about the nature of images, I went on to develop a method that enabled me to
examine them more closely. I found that after retracing a thought sequence, I was able to
cause any one of the component images to arise again. Then, in much the same way as I had
retraced the whole sequence, I could now "retrace" that single image. This
procedure was in fact a kind of insightful looking at the image, seeing it in the new way
that I was now learning to see images: as a slide projected on to whatever was in my field
of vision. When looked at in this way the image would promptly disappear. However, I found
I could then cause it to arise again, and repeat the process. Thus any chosen sample image
could be called forth several times in succession and subjected to repeated insightful
examination.
This repeated examination of single images consolidated my new ability to see images as
images, rather than as the things they depicted. I found it deprived images of their
affective charge. For example, one fairly frequently recurring image, depicting a certain
annoying man who sometimes visited me at the wat, had previously been seen as that
annoying man, and had tended on each occasion to evoke further annoyance. Now it was
seen as an image, and evoked no such reaction. When an image was seen as an image
rather than as the things it depicted, it lost its power to evoke an affective response.
Under such conditions all images were equal. The judgements "annoying,"
attractive," etc., appropriate enough for the contents of images, were not applicable
to the images themselves. The contents carried affective charge; the images themselves, as
mental events, were neutral.
I was reminded of an experience I had had in childhood, when, on looking closely at the
pictures in a newspaper, I had discovered that they were made up of patterns of dots. This
had caused the pictures to assume a very different status. Their content had lost its
appearance of reality and importance. Looked at in that way, as patterns of dots, all the
pictures in the paper acquired a certain sameness; none was better or more interesting
than another. Similarly now, my mental images seen in this new way, as process rather than
content, were in effect all the same. Consequently, any form of affective involvement in
them would have been totally inappropriate.
Under normal circumstances we see images not as images, but as people, places, things,
or whatever else they depict; and we react to them accordingly. Each time I retraced an
image sequence, I realized that I had, up to the moment of retracing, been completely
involved in its content. I realized that under normal circumstances I was as if absorbed
in watching an endless television show. I had first become conscious of the existence of
the mental television show (the endless stream of thought) on the day I had begun
practising concentration, but I had never before realized with what fascination and
emotional involvement I watched it. Now that I had learned to see thought as process
rather than as content, I realized the extent of my former involvement.
It occurred to me that in my normal waking condition I was actually involved in a long,
very realistic dream. I then felt that I understood the significance of a statement I had
come across in my early superficial reading on yoga. The gist of that statement was that,
despite appearances to the contrary, our normal waking state has the quality of a dream.
Just as one sees, on waking in the morning, that one's dream during the night was unreal,
so one will see, on attaining the yogic awakening, that one's former everyday condition
was a waking dream, an illusion, maya. At the time this had made little sense to
me, because I had assumed that it referred to the nature of the physical objects around
us, so that the yogic awakening would presumably reveal those objects as insubstantial and
unreal, more or less as subatomic physics had shown them to be. But now it occurred to me
that the reference must be to the thought-stream. That was the waking dream, the
cosmic illusion, the maya. Previously I had hardly been conscious of its existence, let
alone of its omnipresence and its remarkable power to delude. Now I had seen it in its
true nature -- which, however, did not prevent my being taken in by it again and again.
Insight into its nature had to be renewed each time, by retracing followed optionally by
the more intensive technique of examining individual images.
The shift of attention from content to process resembled the experience of suddenly
realizing, in the middle of watching an absorbing television drama, that it is just a
television drama. Most of the time the drama is perceived as real. One lives it, retracing
with fear, joy, etc., as the plot develops. Then suddenly (perhaps as a result of some
external disturbance) one realizes that the drama is merely a moving pattern of lights on
a glass screen, which one is watching from the armchair on one's living-room. Seeing
images as process rather than as content entails much the same insight, and it produces
the same feeling of having seen through a very realistic illusion.
Continued practice revealed that my earlier model of thought, as made up of pictures
from a mental photograph album, had been an over-simplification. Not all images were
re-presentations of former visual experiences. Some were new combinations of fragments
from several different experiences, as, for example, when I imagined how my room would
look if I were to rearrange the furniture. Others were abstract diagrams, as when I drew
mental graphs to facilitate comprehension of mathematical relationships. But such
constructed images proved to be extremely rare in comparison with simple re-presentations.
The Inner Voice
Another over-simplification that I later realized was the assumption that images --
more precisely, visual images -- were the only elements of thought. I had not suspected
there was any other component present until one day, while engaged in the walking
practice, I had a novel experience which made me realize my error. Having caught myself in
the middle of a train of thought, I found that this had happened because I had been
"stuck for a word" while carrying on a mental conversation; and the reason I was
stuck for a word was that my mental conversation was in Thai, which language I had still
only very imperfectly mastered. Up to that time my introspective examination of thought
sequences had revealed only images. Now, for the first time, I became aware of a second
component: mental verbalizing, "the inner voice." It seemed astonishing that I
had not noticed the inner voice earlier, because it proved thereafter to be a conspicuous
component of the thought-stream, and I knew very well it had been there all along. I
repeatedly found, on retracing an image sequence, that one or more of the images, and
sometimes also the stimulus that had initiated the sequence, were accompanied by mentally
verbalized comments. For example, in the sequence leading from the ache in my back to the
problem of inadequate shelves, the ache was accompanied by the comment, "Bad
posture!" and the final image of the cluttered office by, "How can I work with
stuff everywhere like this?" Usually what I heard was my own voice, as I addressed
some implied listener; and where necessary the language used would switch to suit that
listener. In the case of the interrupted mental conversation in Thai, the listener was a
Thai monk whom I knew well, and whose face was depicted in the accompanying image. Less
often it was the implied other party who was speaking, in which case the inner voice was
generally a partial replay of an earlier actual conversation. With this second layer in
the thought-stream recognized, the resemblance to a slide-show became even closer, for the
slides were now found to be accompanied by a pattering commentary.
Now fully conscious of the thought-stream as a combination of images plus the inner
voice, I soon realized that thought could sometimes be very intrusive. I observed, for
example, how my quiet enjoyment of a magnificent view was marred as soon as the inner
voice began making comments. The effect was similar to that of a noisy group arriving with
a radio playing loudly. The arising of images was equally disruptive. The images,
perceived as if projected on top of the scene before me, obscured my view. They got in the
way of visual perception; I was seeing the view as if through a veil. I found, however,
that the veiling effect could be eliminated by developing concentration. This suppressed
the imagery and verbalizing, yielding a refreshing clarity.
In this way I found the answer to a question I had asked myself during the vipassana
course: What is responsible for the remarkable subjective effects often experienced after
a successful session of concentration practice? What causes the heightened sensory
perception, the feeling of newness in everything, the sense of living in an eternal
present? I now saw it clearly. These subjective effects come about when, by some means,
such as perfecting concentration or becoming absorbed in the beauties of nature, the usual
flow of mental imagery and verbalizing is halted, thus eliminating interference with
incoming sensations. Normally the stream of thought flows on almost incessantly, like an
endless, tiresome television program. Day and night -- except, presumably, in deep sleep
-- sequences of images run on, one after the other, and the inner voice chatters away.
This constant mental activity interferes seriously with perception. We see objects through
an ever-changing veil of images, and hear sounds above the constant mental chatter. But we
are unaware of this because it has always been so. When, through some means, the imagery
and verbalizing are stopped, perception is altered. Colours and shapes, seen directly
without the imagery, appear remarkably vivid. Sounds heard without interference from the
inner voice are heard with great clarity. Objects which under normal circumstances would
evoke associated images (the memories of previous experiences with similar objects), or
comments by the inner voice, are now seen simply as they are, without reference to
previous experience. This produces the sense of newness in everything. Without the
repeatedly flashing images and the constantly droning inner monologue, one is deprived of
an important in-built mental reference for judging temporal duration. Consequently one's
other experiences become timeless; one seems to live in an eternal present moment.
Having recognized how imagery and verbalizing interfered with perception, I thereafter
observed again and again how this phenomenon impaired performance of everyday tasks,
particularly the more mechanical ones. For example, if I was typing, accuracy remained
high as long as imagery was absent and verbalizing was limited to the words actually being
typed. But as soon as some word in the text or some external stimulus set in train a
sequence of imagery, I began making mistakes. When there was imagery, my attention was
divided. I had, as it were, one eye on the text and one eye on the images. I was able to
effect a marked increase in proficiency by consciously suppressing imagery. Almost any
given task could serve in place of mindfulness of breathing or slow walking as the
foundation for concentration -- and for the retracing practice. For example, I would set
about typing with the intention of keeping imagery suppressed; and then whenever an image
sequence did arise, I would retrace it. The retracing caused only a minimal delay because
I was now able to retrace very rapidly, and also because I often caught the first image
before it had time to evoke another and give rise to a full sequence. In this way I
managed to integrate many kinds of daily activity into the practice. Even serious reading
could be done in this way. Like most readers, I was familiar with the experience of
suddenly realizing, on reaching the bottom of a page, that I had little idea what that
page contained, attention having shifted from the content of the page to the content of a
train of imagery. I now made a practice of retracing each such digression before
re-reading. This usually led back to a word on the page that had initiated the digression.
Earlier it had distressed me, both in the vipassana course and during my initial
practice of mindfulness of breathing, that reading and writing, and even serious
conversation, were considered inimical to progress in meditation, and were therefore
banned. Now such activities had become part of my practice. The earlier dichotomy between
practice and everyday life had, to some extent, been broken down.
Positive Value of Imagery and Verbalization
Up to this time imagery and verbalizing had always seemed "undesirable." They
impaired efficiency in the performance of daily tasks; they obscured perception of the
world; and it was they that were responsible for the relatively boring, humdrum quality of
the normal, unconcentrated condition. Certainly imagery and verbalizing, as the objects of
my introspective observation, were the source of all insight. But that insight had so far
revealed no desirable qualities in them.
Increasing integration of the meditation into my daily activities led me gradually to
revise this judgement. On one occasion, as I was setting out from the wat to attend a talk
in the neighbouring town, it suddenly occurred to me that I ought to take a flashlight
with me, as it would be dark when I returned. Retracing revealed the course of mental
events that had led to this useful thought. The beginning point was my seeing a lizard
scamper off into the grass beside the path. Then came a sequence of images depicting the
following: a small snake seen a few days earlier; the old stone steps beside which I had
seen that snake; myself stumbling on those steps while returning to my hut in
semi-darkness the previous night; and, finally, my flashlight on the table in the hut.
This kind of experience was repeated many times. Again and again I observed that image
sequences could be useful.
Another, more sophisticated function of imagery revealed itself during a session of
formal walking practice. I heard a strange yet vaguely familiar sound coming from behind
some trees a short distance to my right. It lasted a few seconds then stopped. The
experience that ensued could be loosely reported as follows. "I could not at first
identify the sound. Then I realized: it was the sound of a load of gravel sliding off the
back of a dump-truck." However, a rigorously phenomenological description would go as
follows. "Initially no image was present, there being nothing in consciousness but
primary sensation, in particular the strange sound. Then there arose a mental image
depicting a dump-truck discharging a load of gravel." Without the image the sound was
just a sound; with the image the sound was identified, recognized. The recognition was
the arising of the image, and vice versa. Such experiences proved common, especially in
conversation. If my partner in a conversation mentioned, for example, the name "Mr.
Somphong," there would arise -- usually -- an image depicting Mr. Somphong as I knew
him. Failure of such an image to arise coincided with partial incomprehension on my part,
a situation of which we might say, "I couldn't recall who Mr. Somphong was."
Similarly in the reverse situation, where I was the one speaking, I found that my words
were usually accompanied by images, and that the words were largely descriptions of what
the images contained.
In this way I noticed more and more frequently that imagery and verbalizing were an
indispensable part of life. This tempered my earlier negative judgement of them. It now
seemed that the question was not how to eliminate imagery and verbalizing, but rather how
to keep them under constant observation so that (a) they would not get out of hand, and
(b) they could be suppressed at a moment's notice if necessary. Increasingly, then, I felt
that what was needed was to perfect the technique of observing images, so that one could
live with them yet not be dominated by them. I did not know how to modify my practice in
order to achieve that. It seemed that the practice of observing images-as-process was as
far as it was possible to go with the technique of retracing. I saw too that retracing had
certain inherent defects and limitations. One defect was that the insight attained through
retracing was always retrospective and intermittent. Retracing brought insight into the
nature of the image sequence, but only after the original sequence, the original mental
event, was already over. I was never aware of the original sequence; I saw only a later
reconstruction of it. Again, retracing always entailed drastic interference with trains of
thought. It entailed, as it were dissecting out sample sections from the flow of thought
and examining them in vitro. I sometimes felt, after retracing, that it would
have been interesting to know what might have come next had the image sequence been
allowed to run on unimpeded.
I could not at that stage see how these defects might be overcome. With such feelings
of mild dissatisfaction about the efficacy of my practice and uncertainty about where to
turn next, I started experimenting with something different. It began as merely a variant
version of retracing, but soon evolved into a new meditation technique.
Link-watching
In my early experimentation with retracing I had noted with interest the
apparent logicality of the order in which images arose. This can be seen in the sample
sequence cited earlier: aching back -to-: slouching pianist -to-: stout
cellist -to-: friend playing cello near shelves -to-: shelves in garage -to-:
office with inadequate shelves. Each link between consecutive images, though by no means
predictable, did seem to be obeying certain "laws." At that time I had not
pursued the matter further because it had seemed relatively unimportant. To examine the
nature of the link between successive images entailed a certain amount of attention to the
content of the images. It had therefore seemed to me that, as regards level of insight,
this was inferior to the practice of viewing images-as-process, whereby one saw beyond the
content to the event. Consequently, in returning to this practice after pursuing retracing
to its limits, I felt I was taking a step backwards; having attained penetrating insight
into the elements of thought, I was now in part relinquishing it.
My investigation into the nature of the links between successive images depended on the
following technique. Suppose a sequence of images A -to-: B -to-: C -to-:
D -to-: E -to-: F. I begin to retrace this sequence, F -to-: E -to-:
D -to-: ... ; but I stop at some arbitrarily chosen image, say the image C, and
permit attention to turn, in a relaxed manner, toward its content. As a result the
sequence tends to resume its original course, as image C is again replaced by image D. But
before it can go further, I again retrace to C. I then repeat, several times over, this
process of alternating between the images C and D, thereby giving myself ample opportunity
to observe the nature of the link C -to-: D.
Repeatedly examining the linking process in this way, I came to perceive the
"laws" that guided it, which, as I later learned from reading in the history of
psychology, were formerly called the "laws of association." However, such
obvious cases as "contiguity in experience" (exemplified in the sequence:
slouching pianist -to-: stout cellist -- the two had been performing together)
and "similarity" (stout cellist -to-: friend playing cello) seemed of
relatively minor importance. Far more important was another process which appeared to be
determined by the images' affective charge. This process is illustrated in the transition
from the image depicting the friend playing his cello near a set of shelves, to the image
depicting a set of shelves in the garage at home. Of the many details of content in the
first of these two images, it is the set of shelves that claims attention and becomes the
cue for the next image; and this clearly reflects a current concern over the problem of
inadequate shelves in my office. Had I not had this problem at the back of my mind, some
other detail of content, such as the friend himself or the tune he was playing, might well
have become the cue for the next image. I repeatedly saw this process operating,
especially toward the end of an image sequence. Near the beginning of a sequence, the
linkages were usually guided by contiguity in experience, similarity, and so on; but as
the sequence developed, the linkages were increasingly guided by current interest or
concern. It followed that the direction my image sequences took was indirectly determined
by my earlier affective involvement in situations, for it was through that involvement
that certain details of content became endowed with their particular emotive charge.
But such discoveries about the mechanism of linking, interesting though they were at
the time, proved in the long term less important than the meditative technique that
yielded them. My practice of causing a pair of consecutive images to re-arise alternately
several times over, entailed two distinctly different phases. Phase 1 was simply a special
case of retracing, going back from image D to image C, going upstream against the natural
flow of thought. Phase 2 was the reverse of this. It was, in effect, a re-enactment of the
original mental event: image C was allowed to link again to image D, more or less as had
happened in the original thought sequence. It was a relaxed downstream movement, a going
along with the natural flow of thought. Repeatedly practising these two phases in rapid
succession, I became very conscious of the difference between them. Phase 1 entailed
effort and attention, and yielded penetrating insight into the nature of image-as-process.
Phase 2 entailed relaxation and diminution of attention, and a partial switch of focus
from image-as-process to image-as-content. However, care had to be taken to ensure that
the relaxation was only partial, that the diminution of attention did not go too far. If I
let go completely, the second image would link to another image, and another, and the flow
of thought would resume without insight. I therefore learned to relax attention just
sufficiently to permit a single linkage to take place. The result was a delicately
balanced form of insight which saw image-as-process and image-as-content simultaneously.
In time I abandoned phase 1 and developed phase 2 as a technique in its own right. I
found that by preserving the delicate balance, I could move with the stream of thought,
observing successive images as they re-arose in the forward direction, always seeing
image-as-process and image-as-content simultaneously. It was therefore possible, after
retracing a full image sequence, to observe it without interference as it then repeated
its original forward movement. The earlier technique of retracing had involved a cycle of
three stages; with the perfecting of this new technique, a fourth stage was added:
1. concentration on a chosen object;
2. a short thought sequence, as the mind wanders from the object;
3. retracing this thought sequence to its starting point;
4. watching the same thought sequence with insight, link by link, as it again moves in
the forward direction.
This new technique (stage 4) overcame what seemed to me a major defect of retracing
(stage 3). With retracing it had only been possible to observe thought sequences in
reverse order. With this new technique it was possible to observe each thought sequence a
second time as it again moved in the natural forward direction. It remained true, however,
that what was revealed was not the original unimpeded flow of thought. It was still a
reconstructed version of a sample sequence dissected out of that flow. Further refinement
was needed.
Awareness
In the event, the required refinement in technique came of its own accord
without being sought. I found that when practising stage 4, the mind had a tendency to run
on after arriving at the end of each sample sequence. Thus an original sequence A -to-:
B -to-: C -to-: D -to-: E -to-: F, after being
retraced and observed as it moved again in the forward direction, often would not stop on
reaching image F, but would continue into a new sequence: -to-: F -to-:
G -to-: H -to-: .... Provided I maintained the delicately balanced
insight of stage 4, I could have the continuing thought sequence without losing awareness
of its true nature as process. I was therefore observing thought continuously, as it
happened. Images arose one after the other, sometimes in very rapid succession, sometimes
slowly. In themselves, and in the manner of their linking together, they were as I had
come to know them in the earlier practices. But now, instead of looking at re-plays of
artificially isolated segments, I was observing the original, undisturbed process itself.
The inner voice was also clearly heard. I listened as it made its intermittent comments,
or at times took over as the dominant component of thought. I was now listening in on, and
watching, the processes of thought while they were going on, and without interfering with
them. This, I was certain, was the ultimate in insight, the ideal technique in insight
meditation. To refer to it I later adopted the term used by some of its best-known
practitioners and advocates: awareness.
The only defect in awareness, as I was practising it, was that I usually could not
maintain it for more than half a minute at a time. The collapse of awareness coincided
with, and indeed was identical with, losing sight of the process and becoming involved
again in the content. Whenever awareness broke down in this way, I was able to
re-establish it by again going through the lead-up stages of retracing and link-watching.
However, as I became more familiar with the practice, I found I could dispense with those
preliminaries, and establish awareness directly. I therefore lived in a continual
alternation between two different conditions: awareness and unawareness. Awareness would
last until the mind reverted -- through a kind of fatigue, it seemed -- to its normal
unaware condition. That condition would then last until something, usually difficult to
identify, reminded me that I ought to be practising -- whereupon I would re-establish
awareness.
I found that awareness could be practised in any situation, regardless of what
activity, physical or mental, I was engaged in. This was to be expected, since awareness
coexists with the flow of thought and in no way impedes it. Even intensive study and
complicated problem-solving activities proved compatible with the practice of awareness.
Indeed, awareness seemed to contribute to greater efficiency, by enabling me to notice
immediately any irrelevant digression and correct it.
However, it was not for these practical benefits that I valued awareness, but for the
unobscured insight which it yielded into the ordinary everyday working of the mind. It was
as if the dark, mysterious room of my mind, into which the earlier practices had
occasionally sent flashes of light, was now completely lit up for lengthy periods. At the
level of process, what I saw was an essentially simple, orderly mechanism. Images, drawn
from the vast mental photograph album of memory, appeared one after the other, usually
according to the principles of linking already observed (but sometimes spontaneously and
randomly), while the inner voice kept up its commentary. At the level of content, much of
what I saw could be best described as useless. It seemed that most of the images stored in
my mental album were of little intrinsic value, and the affective charges inhering in them
were such that the most useless images were the ones likely to arise most frequently.
However, my only response on seeing all this was detached amusement. Observing the antics
of my mind often evoked a smile. This detached attitude was not cultivated. It was the
only one possible, since affective involvement in the content of thought always caused
awareness to collapse. Awareness and affective involvement were incompatible. For example,
I could observe with awareness and detachment an image that would normally be conducive to
an angry reaction; or I could react angrily to that image and loose awareness; but I could
not do both at once. Awareness could not coexist with anger or with any other such
reaction. Awareness entailed detachment. It seemed as if the mental energy normally
squandered in emotional reaction to images was now being deployed instead as awareness.
The Nature of the Self
While there was much of interest in the details of content and process revealed
under the spotlight of awareness, it all seemed of minor importance compared with one
over-riding insight which constituted the very essence of the experience. That insight
provided the answer to a number of interrelated questions, some of which had preoccupied
me since my earliest encounter with meditation: Who -- or what -- is doing the thinking?
What is it that is aware? Who is observing all these processes? What is the nature of this
observing?
It seemed natural to describe awareness with the statement, "I am observing the
mental processes." But at the same time, that statement was self-evidently
misleading. It suggested a situation analogous to that of a spectator watching a street
parade, which was not at all the situation that existed in awareness. It was not a case of
an observer, "I," engaged in observing a spectacle, "the mental
processes." Rather -- and this was the central insight of awareness -- the observer was
the mental processes. Instead of saying, "I am observing the mental processes,"
one ought to say, "I am the mental processes." In fact I verbalized
this startling revelation in more or less the following words: "Good grief, this is me!
I'm all this!" The supposed observer was identical with the object being
observed. And the same was true of the supposed act of observing: the observing of the
mental processes was nothing other than those processes. Observer, observed, and observing
were all one and the same.
The moment awareness broke down, this vision was lost. Without awareness the split
returned; there was again the sense of being "I, the thinker"; and there were
again the mental contents or thoughts that I was thinking. While there was no awareness,
there existed the feeling of being an "I" or thinker, separate and distinct from
the thoughts. But when there was awareness, this feeling vanished, and it became
self-evident that there was no "I" or thinker separate from the thinking
process. The feeling of being an "I" separate from the thoughts coincided with
failure to see thoughts as process. The seeming reality of "I, the thinker"
coincided with the seeming reality of the contents of images. The two arose and ceased
together, as two aspects of the same fundamental illusion.
Awareness was, I was certain, the ultimate meditative technique. As long as awareness
was maintained, there was uninterrupted insight into the functioning of the mind. It was
not exactly the case that that functioning was not thereby interfered with. Clearly,
mental events did not go on just as they would have done in the absence of awareness.
Awareness implied recognition of the true nature of images, and hence absence of any
affective involvement in their contents; and this would certainly influence the direction
taken by thought sequences. Nevertheless, awareness was a maximally non-intrusive
technique for observing the thought-stream in operation.
In retrospect I saw that my four years of meditative effort had in one respect led me
in a circle. I had begun by attempting to stop the thought-stream (through concentration),
then I had tried making it go backwards (retracing), and finally I had let it flow
normally again (link-watching, awareness). The mind had thus arrived back at its
starting-point. However, in the process it had acquired an important new skill: it had
learned to be aware of itself in action.
As for the way ahead, that now seemed clear and beyond doubt: awareness must be
perfected. The intermittent, brief periods of awareness must become progressively more
frequent and must last progressively longer. Other techniques learned earlier in my
meditative career still had their place; basic concentration, together with general
mindfulness of the body, feelings, and emotional states, would always be valuable as a
foundation. However, awareness of the thought-stream was the real practice. The practice
would be perfected when the mind had become fully and uninterruptedly aware of itself. The
achievement of that condition would surely be the culmination of the entire meditative
endeavour.
***
[Source: Roderick S. Bucknell, "Experiments in Insight Meditation," Australian
Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 3 (1983), 96-117. Republished in The
Meditative Way: Readings in the theory and practice of Buddhist meditation, edited by
Rod Bucknell and Chris Kang (Richmond: Curzon, 1997).]
***
Sincere thanks to Ti.nh
Tue^. for providing this article.